Loading...
News Article

Some LEDs look on the brighter side of white

Tests have inicated that under halogen light and violet-pumped LED lights, the whiteness of cards (of varying whiteness) can be correctly assessed. On the other hand, the human eye cannot correctly differentiate whiteness when exposed to the most common type of modules used in LED lamps, blue pumped LEDs
For years, companies have been adding whiteners to laundry detergent, paints, plastics, paper and fabrics to make whites look "whiter than white."

Now, with a switch away from incandescent and fluorescent lighting, different degrees of whites may all look the same, according to experts in lighting.

"Retailers have long been concerned with the colour-rendering qualities of their lighting, but less aware how light sources render white," says Kevin W. Houser, professor of architectural engineering, Penn State.



Kevin Houser, Professor of Architectural Engineering at Penn State, sorts tiles in a light box in the departments illuminating engineering lab for observation under several light sources (Image: Patrick Mansell)

Not long ago, the only practical choices for home, office or commercial lighting were incandescent or fluorescent bulbs. More recently, compact fluorescent bulbs, which use less energy than incandescent bulbs, became popular, but compact fluorescents are not always accepted by consumers because of poor colour rendition, lack of dimability, slow warm-up to full output and because they contain mercury.

The most recent popular entry into home or commercial lighting are LED bulbs, which while currently expensive, are often even more energy-saving than compact fluorescents.

While some LED bulbs will make colours pop, the vast majority do not showcase or differentiate the appearance of white products, according to Houser, because all white light is not the same.

Different light sources contain different combinations of the wavelengths of light. A broad variety of wavelengths will create light that appears white to the human eye, but different mixtures of wavelengths will affect how colours are rendered. When it comes to seeing the colour white, the light source is very important because of how product manufacturers make white products appear white using whiteners.

Whiteners contain fluorescent materials that glow under violet and ultraviolet light. Sunlight, fluorescent light and incandescent light all produce some light in the violet and ultraviolet range. The whiteners used in consumer products work under those conditions, resulting in a bright white perception.

However, most current LED bulbs use blue LEDs to excite a phosphor that then glows white, but produces no violet or ultraviolet light.



A rainbow pattern as seen under a long-linear, fluorescent light source common for commercial interiors (top). And, the same rainbow pattern as it appears under a non-commercial, fluorescent light source (bottom). The colour differences result from the use of different phosphors in the lights glass tubing (Image: Patrick Mansell)

Houser, working with a Penn State student and researchers from Soraa of Fremont, California, asked thirty-nine participants to observe various combinations of light sources and white objects to see how light source affected perceptions of white. They report their results in a recent issue of Leukos, the Journal of the Illuminating Engineering Society.

The participants completed three tests - selection, forced choice and sorting - using five different light sources - a blue-pumped LED, filtered halogen lamp and three violet-pumped LEDs with differing levels of violet emissions.

In the sorting experiment, the researchers placed six calibrated whiteness cards of varying whiteness on a table in a booth enclosed on three sides. They asked participants to arrange the cards in order of whiteness under each of the five light sources.

Under the halogen light and violet-pumped LED lights with 7 and 11 percent violet emission, the order was correct. Two of the cards were flipped under violet-pumped LEDs with only three percent violet emissions.

"With the LED with only blue pumping the phosphors, the order became random," says Houser. "People simply couldn't tell the difference between the cards under the blue-pumped LED, which is notable because blue-pumped LEDs are by far the most common type for general lighting."

In the forced choice test, two nominally identical cards were placed in each of two booths containing different light sources. Participants were asked to choose the card that was whiter under all of the permutations of each of the five light sources.



The same set of white cards as seen under a violet-pumped light emitting diode (LED) top, and under a blue-pumped LED below (Image: Patrick Mansell)

"The light sources with higher violet component permitted the best discrimination between the targets," adds Houser.

In the selection test, researchers asked the participants to look at a reference card in one booth and rank the cards in a second booth as either as white or whiter than the reference card. Again the blue-pumped LEDs did not fare well.

The researchers note that "engineering of an LED source's spectrum is necessary for an accurate rendering of whiteness."

These results have been described in further detail in the paper, " Whiteness Perception under LED Illumination," by Kevin W. Houser et al in LEUKOS: The Journal of the Illuminating Engineering Society of North America, 10, (3), p165 - 80, 2014.

DOI:  10.1080/15502724.2014.902750


Soraa Inc. funded this study.

×
Search the news archive

To close this popup you can press escape or click the close icon.
Logo
x
Logo
×
Register - Step 1

You may choose to subscribe to the Compound Semiconductor Magazine, the Compound Semiconductor Newsletter, or both. You may also request additional information if required, before submitting your application.


Please subscribe me to:

 

You chose the industry type of "Other"

Please enter the industry that you work in:
Please enter the industry that you work in: